Which hemisphere recognizes faces




















J Neurosci. The improbable simplicity of the fusiform face area. Trends Cogn Sci. We experience these things every day, but how do our brains create them? Your Brain, Explained is a personal tour around your gray matter. Louis , author, Origins of Neuroscience. Dingman weaves classic studies with modern research into easily digestible sections, to provide an excellent primer on the rapidly advancing field of neuroscience.

View fullsize. Receive New Content by Email. Order Now. Indeed, there has been substantial consensus on his point. One of the goals of our studies has been to examine the extent to which these systems are truly independent.

The motivation for the investigations grew out of our observation that in most, if not all, functional imaging studies, word-selective and face-selective brain activation is usually present in both hemispheres is bilateral rather than just in a single hemisphere unilateral and that, interestingly, the sites of activation are the same across the two hemispheres. Our findings suggest that word and face representations are not as independent as previously thought.

From a clinical perspective, this is also an interesting avenue for research. For example, damage to each hemisphere affects the perception of both words and faces and difficulty in acquiring mastery over say word recognition is associated with reduced use of just one hemisphere for face representations.

Taken together, the mechanisms used for face and word recognition form part of a more complex, dynamic system, which, over the course of development, comes to be optimized for rapid and efficient recognition of complex visual inputs. Behrmann: We know that in adults, the left hemisphere is better than the right hemisphere of the brain in recognizing words. This is probably because in the majority of the population, language systems are more left- than right-sided. We were particularly interested in exploring how this mature hemispheric profile emerges over development or whether it is present from a very young age.

We conducted a half-field study in which we presented subjects with words or faces in each visual field, on the right or left. We showed that adults evinced the complementary superiority of faces in the left visual field right hemisphere and words in the right visual field left hemisphere. Although adolescents were overall as accurate as adults, they only showed a left hemisphere advantage for words and no hemisphere difference for faces.

The same was true for younger children, although their overall accuracy was lower than either of the other groups. These findings suggest that the division between the hemispheres in visual recognition for words precedes that for faces.

We also showed that the extent to which a participant was a proficient reader was predictive of specialization of face perception on the right hemisphere.

Kikuchi, M. Altered brain connectivity in 3-to 7-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. Neuroimage Clin. Kis, A. The Way Dogs Canis familiaris look at human emotional faces is modulated by oxytocin. An eye-tracking study. Kita, Y. Self-face recognition in children with autism spectrum disorders: a near-infrared spectroscopy study.

Brain Dev. Kleinhans, N. Abnormal functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders during face processing. Brain , — Kosaka, H. Oxytocin efficacy is modulated by dosage and oxytocin receptor genotype in young adults with high-functioning autism: a week randomized clinical trial.

Psychiatry 6:e Neural substrates participating in acquisition of facial familiarity: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 20, — Differential effects of oxytocin on social sensitivity in two distinct breeds of dogs Canis familiaris. Psychoneuroendocrinology 74, — Krause, B. Can transcranial electrical stimulation improve learning difficulties in atypical brain development? A future possibility for cognitive training.

Kumazaki, H. Android robot-mediated mock job interview sessions for young adults with autism spectrum disorder: a pilot study. Psychiatry Lerer, E. Low CD38 expression in lymphoblastoid cells and haplotypes are both associated with autism in a family-based study. Autism Res. Lin, Y. Oxytocin stimulates hippocampal neurogenesis via oxytocin receptor expressed in CA3 pyramidal neurons.

Liu, N. Optical-imaging-based neurofeedback to enhance therapeutic intervention in adolescents with autism: methodology and initial data. Neurophotonics Lopatina, O.

The roles of oxytocin and CD38 in social or parental behaviors. Lukas, M. Oxytocin mediates rodent social memory within the lateral septum and the medial amygdala depending on the relevance of the social stimulus: male juvenile versus female adult conspecifics. Macchitella, L. Oxytocin improves the ability of dogs to follow informative pointing: a neuroemotional hypothesis. Lincei 28, — Madrid, J.

Preference for novel faces in male infant monkeys predicts cerebrospinal fluid oxytocin concentrations later in life. Marusak, H. Amygdala responses to salient social cues vary with oxytocin receptor genotype in youth. Neuropsychologia 79, 1—9.

McMahon, D. Face-selective neurons maintain consistent visual responses across months. McQuaid, R. Mul, J. Neuropsychopharmacology doi: Munesue, T. Oxytocin for male subjects with autism spectrum disorder and comorbid intellectual disabilities: a randomized pilot study. Nakajima, M. Oxytocin modulates female sociosexual behavior through a specific class of prefrontal cortical interneurons. Cell , — Nelson, C. The development and neural bases of face recognition.

Infant Child Dev. Neumann, I. Brain oxytocin: a key regulator of emotional and social behaviours in both females and males. Ohayon, S. What makes a cell face selective? Neuron 74, — Oliva, J. Oxytocin enhances the appropriate use of human social cues by the domestic dog Canis familiaris in an object choice task. Patriquin, M. Neuroanatomical and neurofunctional markers of social cognition in autism spectrum disorder: social brain in autism. Pessoa, L. Target visibility and visual awareness modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces.

Cortex 16, — Pierce, K. Brain J. Proverbio, A. Sex differences in social cognition: the case of face processing. Qi, C. Interaction of basolateral amygdala, ventral hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex regulates the consolidation and extinction of social fear.

Brain Funct. Quiroga, R. Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature , — Raam, T. Hippocampal oxytocin receptors are necessary for discrimination of social stimuli. Ramon, M. Neural microgenesis of personally familiar face recognition. Remedios, R. Social behaviour shapes hypothalamic neural ensemble representations of conspecific sex. Rimmele, U. Oxytocin makes a face in memory familiar. Rogers, C.

Oxytocin- and arginine vasopressin-containing fibers in the cortex of humans, chimpanzees, and rhesus macaques. Sabihi, S. Oxytocin in the medial prefrontal cortex attenuates anxiety: anatomical and receptor specificity and mechanism of action. Neuropharmacology , 1— Oxytocin in the prelimbic medial prefrontal cortex reduces anxiety-like behavior in female and male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 45, 31— Scalaidhe, S. Face-selective neurons during passive viewing and working memory performance of rhesus monkeys: evidence for intrinsic specialization of neuronal coding.

Cortex 9, — Shin, N. Effects of intranasal oxytocin on emotion recognition in korean male: a dose-response study. Psychiatry Investig doi: Simons, J. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory.

Skuse, D. Common polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene OXTR is associated with human social recognition skills. Slane, M. Social cognition, face processing, and oxytocin receptor single nucleotide polymorphisms in typically developing children. Tang, J. Face recognition and visual search strategies in autism spectrum disorders: amending and extending a recent review by Weigelt et al.

Todorov, A. The role of the amygdala in face perception and evaluation. Social judgments from faces. Tsao, D. Typically, divided field studies of face recognition involve the use of peripherally viewed faces Dundas et al. Neural populations in the right hemisphere — such as the FFA — associated with specialized face processing e.

The FFA has been implicated in holistic processing of spatial relations Rossion et al. Whether or not the LVF bias for face-processing mechanisms in the right hemisphere sufficiently explains the LVF half-face advantage is less clear. A combined behavioral and neuroimaging study by Yovel, Tambini and Brandman explicitly implicates the FFA as underlying the LVF half-face advantage, which highlights the possibility of a common underlying mechanism for LVF advantage reported for both peripherally and centrally viewed faces.

This means that, like the LVF advantage for peripherally viewed whole faces, the LVF half-face advantage may too involve an LVF advantage, albeit closer to the center of gaze.

Despite the plausibility of a common mechanism underlying the LVF bias for faces viewed centrally and peripherally, the results of our experiments complicate this view. In our study, we found that for half-faces viewed alone, in either the RVF or the LVF, equivalent recognition accuracy and inversion effects occurred in each location. We interpret this as indicative of equivalent processing of spatial relations for RVF and LVF half-faces viewed in isolation.

Our finding of equivalent processing for RVF and LVF halves of centrally viewed faces when viewed alone suggests that greater face-processing resources are available for centrally viewed faces as compared to peripherally viewed faces. That is, in contrast to the LVF advantage for peripherally viewed faces, the LVF half-face bias may arise as the result of a limitation in applying face-processing resources simultaneously to hemifield-split half-faces rather than an inherent deficit of processing of RVF face stimuli by the LH.

It is plausible that the presence of an upright LVF half-face biases this competition by allocating face-processing resources to the LVF at the expense of RVF face processing, even though equivalent half-face processing is possible for the RVF half of a face when viewed alone. Their results are consistent with ours from Experiments 2 b and 3 , for which we observed no differences in recognition performance, but the equivalent LVF and RVF holistic processing they observed contrasts with our finding of an LVF advantage for centrally viewed upright whole faces.

However, the paradigm they used depended on cuing either LVF or RVF half-faces, which would be consistent with the biasing of attention toward the LVF or RVF by the presence of only one half face Experiment 2 b or the pairing of an upright and inverted half-face Experiment 3. A resource allocation explanation would thus reconcile the results of our experiments with those of Liu et al. Our experiments did not manipulate attention, so we can only speculate about a potential role of attention in our results.

A possible role of visual attention has been acknowledged in previous studies of the LVF half-face advantage because it exhibits an inherent LVF bias for faces and non-face stimuli J. If so, the prioritization of LVF face processing would reflect a means of optimizing allocation of limited face-processing resources, especially those in the RH.

Whether prioritization of LVF half-face processing is the product of a learned attention mechanism, or an alternative mechanism, we propose that it is necessary to account for the LVF half-face bias, in addition to the coupling of RH superiority and LVF-RH correspondence. This proposal is also relevant to similarities and differences between the LVF half-face advantage and the LVF advantage for whole faces.

We also note that the LVF advantage occurs for single whole faces viewed in the periphery Dundas et al. We conclude that RH superiority alone is not sufficient to explain the LVF advantage in face recognition, and that an additional condition, the prioritization of an upright LVF half-face over a simultaneously viewed upright half-face in the RVF, is necessary to account for the LVF half-face bias.

Bartlett, J. What are the routes to face recognition? Rhodes Eds. Perception of faces, objects, and scenes: Analytic and holistic processes pp.

New York: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar. Barton, J. Discrimination of spatial relations and features in faces: Effects of inversion and viewing duration. British Journal of Psychology , 92 3 , — Article PubMed Google Scholar. Beaumont, J. Methods for Studying Cerebral Hemispheric Function. In Functions of the Right Cerebral Hemisphere pp.

Boremanse, A. An objective signature for visual binding of face parts in the human brain. Journal of Vision , 13 11 , 6—6. Dissociation of part-based and integrated neural responses to faces by means of electroencephalographic frequency tagging. European Journal of Neuroscience , 40 6 , — Bourne, V. The divided visual field paradigm: Methodological considerations.

Laterality , 11 4 , — Chimeric faces, visual field bias, and reaction time bias: Have we been missing a trick? Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition , 13 1 , 92— Article Google Scholar. Examining the effects of inversion on lateralisation for processing facial emotion. Cortex , 47 6 , — Brady, N. Perceptual asymmetries are preserved in memory for highly familiar faces of self and friend. Brain and Cognition , 58 3 , — Brainard, D. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision , 10 4 , — Bukowski, H.

Cerebral lateralization of face-sensitive areas in left-handers: Only the FFA does not get it right. Cortex , 49 9 , — Burt, D. Perceptual asymmetries in judgements of facial attractiveness, age, gender, speech and expression. Neuropsychologia , 35 5 , — Butler, S.

Does inversion abolish the left chimeric face processing advantage? Neuroreport , 16 18 , — Cattaneo, Z. Hemispheric asymmetry in discriminating faces differing for featural or configural second-order relations aspects.

Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , 21 2 , — Cheung, O. Chua, K. Category-specific learned attentional bias to object parts. Becoming a lunari or taiyo expert: Learned attention to parts drives holistic processing of faces.

Holistic processing from learned attention to parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 4 , — Cohen, J. Statistical power for the social sciences. Collishaw, S. Perception , 29 8 , — Coolican, J. Perceptual biases in processing facial identity and emotion.

Brain and Cognition , 66 2 , — Diamond, R. Why faces are and are not special: An effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 2 , — Dundas, E. The joint development of hemispheric lateralization for words and faces.

Journal of Experimental Psychology. General , 2 , — Variable left-hemisphere language and orthographic lateralization reduces right-hemisphere face lateralization. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 27 5 , — Farah, M. What causes the face inversion effect?

Faul, F. Behavior Research Methods , 41 4 , — Freire, A. The face-inversion effect as a deficit in the encoding of configurai information: Direct evidence. Perception , 29 2 , — Gauthier, I. Nature Neuroscience , 2 6 , — Gentile, F. Neural competition through visual similarity in face selection. Brain Research , , — Temporal dynamics of face selection mechanism in the context of similar and dissimilar faces: ERP evidence for biased competition within the ventral occipito-temporal cortex using ICA.

NeuroImage , 59 1 , — Gilbert, C. Visual asymmetry in perception of faces. Neuropsychologia , 11 3 , — Goffaux, V. Hasson, U. Neuron , 34 3 , — Heller, W.

Perception and expression of emotion in right-handers and left-handers. Neuropsychologia , 19 2 , — Hellige, J. Effects of perceptual quality on the processing of human faces presented to the left and right cerebral hemispheres. Brain and Cognition , 7 1 , 39— Hemond, C. Hillger, L. Separable mechanisms in face processing: Evidence from hemispheric specialization.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 3 1 , 42— Hilliard, R. Cortex , 9 3 , — Hole, G. Configurational factors in the perception of unfamiliar faces.

Perception , 23 1 , 65— Hsiao, J. Two Fixations Suffice in Face Recognition.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000